• Users Online: 2309
  • Home
  • Print this page
  • Email this page
Home Current issue Archives Ahead of print Search Subscribe Instructions Submit article About us Editorial board Contacts Login 

 Table of Contents  
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Year : 2019  |  Volume : 40  |  Issue : 2  |  Page : 114-122

Auditory P300 and neuropsychological cognitive functioning assessment of patients with chronic hepatitis B and C infection


1 Department of Neuropsychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, South Valley University, Qena, Egypt
2 Department of Neuropsychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Sohag University, Sohag, Egypt

Date of Submission04-Feb-2019
Date of Acceptance17-Feb-2019
Date of Web Publication11-Jul-2019

Correspondence Address:
Tarek Desoky
Department of Neuropsychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, South Valley University, Qena, 83511
Egypt
Login to access the Email id

Source of Support: None, Conflict of Interest: None


DOI: 10.4103/ejpsy.ejpsy_6_19

Rights and Permissions
  Abstract 


Background A growing body of interest was addressed to the concept of minimal rather than overt hepatic encephalopathy as a subclinical manifestation in chronic hepatitis B- and C-infected patients with normal liver function. This study aims to neuropsychologically and neurophysiologically assess those patients for minimal cognitive impairment and its correlates.
Patients and methods Thorough neuropsychological evaluation including Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Trail Making Test, Montreal Cognitive Assessment Test, and Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), and neurophysiological (P300-evoked potential) assessment were conducted on untreated blood donors with hepatitis C virus (n=24) and hepatitis B virus (n=16) infection and 40 healthy controls.
Results All tools of assessment showed a significant difference between cases and controls that suggests higher incidence of depression and cognitive dysfunction among hepatitis patients with no difference between B and C types of infection apart from BDI, which showed higher scores (more depressive features) among hepatitis C rather than B. Among variables such as patient age, BMI, BDI, and viral load titer, only BDI showed a significant positive correlation with such impairments.
Conclusion Neuropsychological- and neurophysiological-based evidence of cognitive impairment was proved to be significantly present among cases with chronic hepatitis B and C infection. The value of such evaluation in the management plan of such group of patients needs further future studies.

Keywords: cognitive impairment, depression, hepatitis B virus, hepatitis C virus


How to cite this article:
Desoky T, Baddary H, Moneim MA, Ali A. Auditory P300 and neuropsychological cognitive functioning assessment of patients with chronic hepatitis B and C infection. Egypt J Psychiatr 2019;40:114-22

How to cite this URL:
Desoky T, Baddary H, Moneim MA, Ali A. Auditory P300 and neuropsychological cognitive functioning assessment of patients with chronic hepatitis B and C infection. Egypt J Psychiatr [serial online] 2019 [cited 2024 Mar 29];40:114-22. Available from: https://new.ejpsy.eg.net//text.asp?2019/40/2/114/262553




  Introduction Top


Chronic hepatitis represents a major health problem and a main cause of liver disease in Egypt and the whole world. Egypt has the highest prevalence of hepatitis C infection in the world (Blach et al., 2017) with a prevalence of 10% among adults aged from 15 to 59 years in the Egyptian Ministry of Health Survey in 2015 (Ministry of Health and Population Egypt, El-Zanaty and Associates Egypt & and ICF International, 2015), while hepatitis B showed a lower prevalence at 1.7% in Egypt (Owiti et al., 2015).

Cognitive dysfunction in cases of decompensated liver cirrhosis complicating chronic hepatitis is understandable by the effect of liver impairment on the brain functioning as being demonstrated in chronic hepatic encephalopathy (Córdoba et al., 2003).

However, cases of chronic hepatitis with normal liver functioning showed some element of subtle cognitive impairment which might affect one-third of chronic hepatitis C virus (HCV) infection cases without decompensated liver cirrhosis (Monaco et al., 2012).

The concept of minimal rather than overt hepatic encephalopathy as a complication of chronic liver disease had been highlighted in many researches in the last years. Minimal hepatic encephalopathy (MHE) has its impact on the quality of life and fate of illness of such patients and hence, it had been focused and received much concern in research. MHE was defined as the test-dependent mental dysfunction while no clinical evidence of overt hepatic encephalopathy is being existent (Vilstrup et al., 2014).


  Aim of the work Top


This study aimed to investigate a group of HCV and hepatitis B virus (HBV) infected patients who did not undergo any current or previous specific antiviral treatment for cognitive dysfunction and depression, to compare chronic hepatitis C and hepatitis B and to define the possible correlates of cognitive functioning in such group of patients.


  Patients and methods Top


Study population

The study involved a total of 40 consecutive patients recruited from the blood donors at the blood bank of Sohag University Hospital (de novo nontreated cases). The participants were grouped into three categories: group a included 24 patients having the diagnosis of chronic hepatitis C infection, group b included 16 patients having the diagnosis of chronic hepatitis B infection, and group c included age-matched and sex-matched 40 healthy controls for correlation. The study was conducted during a 1-year period from March 2015 to March 2016. An informed written consent was obtained from all participants for participation in the study and the rules of the ethics committee of Sohag University had been respected.

The inclusion criteria included de novo (no current or previous treatment with interferon), uncomplicated (Child–Pugh score class A) HCV and HBV patients, aged (20–50) years and the exclusion criteria included complicated cases (Child–Pugh score class B/C), concomitant HIV infection, current or previous interferon therapy, neurological or psychiatric, illicit or psychotropic drug use, and relevant concomitant medial illnesses (renal failure, cardiac failure, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, hypothyroidism, or uncontrolled diabetes).

Methods

The following tools were performed on all participants under the study: (a) thorough general and neurological examination for exclusion of medical and neurological disorders that might represent a cause of cognitive impairment; (b) blood tests included complete blood count, random blood glucose (RBG), renal function tests, liver function tests, HBV surface antigens, anti-HCV antibodies, PCR viral titer, HIV serology, and thyroid-stimulating hormone; (c) pelvi-abdominal ultrasonography was performed for assessment of the liver and exclusion of cirrhosis or marked fibrosis; (d) Beck Depression Inventory (BDI): a self-reported 21-item questionnaire widely used to assess severity of depressive symptoms, 0–3 choices for each item, with a score of more than or equal to 10 considered abnormal; 10–20 mild, 20–30 moderate, and more than 30 severe depression (Beck et al., 1996); (e) neuropsychological cognitive battery which comprised (i) Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised, short form (WMS-R): a widely used psychometric tool for assessment of the various aspects of memory in adults. It includes a group of subtests that evaluates the different areas of memory and learning. These subtests are figural memory, logical memory, visual paired associate I, visual reproduction I, and visual memory span. It has five summary scores which are: general memory, verbal memory, visual memory, attention/concentration, and delayed recall (Butters et al., 1988). (ii) Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) is a widely used psychometric tool for the assessment of executive functioning which was first developed by Grant and Berg (1948). The participants are asked to sort and switch a group of cards that are typed according to their color, shape, and number. Perseverative error is considered when the previous rule is employed incorrectly (Heaton et al., 1993). Executive functions tested by the WCST include: sequencing, planning, decision-making, ability to form abstract concepts, and working memory (Lezak et al., 2012). We used the computerized form of the test which is available online (PEBL WCST). (iii) Trail Making Test (TMT-A and TMT-B): is a wide-spectrum cognitive tool that addresses visual attention, speed of processing, mental flexibility, and executive functioning, which is formed of two parts, TMT-A of 25 circles of consecutive numbers from 1 to 25 and TMT-B of 24 circles of A–L letters and 1–12 numbers. The test taker is asked to match numbers in TMT-A and number to letter in TMT-B and timing the task in each. Average score for parts A and B are 29 and 78 s, respectively, and considered deficient if more than 75 and 273 s, respectively (Reitan, 1958). (iv) Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MOCA) test: is a screening test for dementia and sensitive for mild cognitive impairment and considered more sensitive than the more popular Mini-Mental State Examination. It measures attention, memory, orientation, language, executive function, naming, visuospatial orientation, and abstraction. The total score is 30 points divided into separate subscores and less than 26 points is considered abnormal (Nasreddine et al., 2003, 2005). (f) Auditory P300-evoked brain potential: an event-related potential of cognitive neurophysiological significance. It represents the brain response to certain sensory/cognitive stimuli by paying attention to the different stimulus among the stereotypical ones (the auditory oddball paradigm). It measures mainly the decision-making process (Polich and Kok, 1995).

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis of data was done using the IBM SPSS software, version 24. Data were displayed as frequency with the percentage for categorical data and mean with SD for numerical data. Independent Student’s t test was used to compare the means of two groups. For testing the relationship between variables we used Pearson’s correlation coefficient. The P value was considered significant if less than 0.05 with 95% confidence interval.


  Results Top


Demographic and clinical data

Twenty-four patients with HCV infection and 16 patients with HBV infection, collected from the blood donors attending the Blood Bank Unit at Sohag University Hospital and 40 age-matched and sex-matched healthy controls participated this study. Demographic and clinical data of cases and controls are shown in [Table 1].
Table 1 Demographic and clinical data of the study groups

Click here to view


Beck depression inventory

Twelve (50%) patients with HCV and six (37.5%) patients with HBV had a BDI score of more than or equal to 10 (significant depression). There was a significant difference suggesting higher depression scores in cases than in controls and in HCV than in HBV ([Table 2]).
Table 2 Diagnosis of depression among cases and controls

Click here to view


Neuropsychological assessment

All subtests of the WMS-R, WCST, TMT-A and TMT-B, and MOCA test showed significant lower scores in cases compared with controls suggesting cognitive impairment among chronic hepatitis patients with no significant difference between hepatitis C and B groups ([Table 3],[Table 4],[Table 5],[Table 6],[Table 7],[Table 8],[Table 9]).
Table 3 Wechsler Memory Scale in patients and controls

Click here to view
Table 4 Wechsler Memory Scale in hepatitis B virus and hepatitis C virus patients

Click here to view
Table 5 Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in patients and controls

Click here to view
Table 6 Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in between hepatitis B virus and hepatitis C virus patients

Click here to view
Table 7 Trail Making Test and Montreal Cognitive Assessment Test between patients and controls

Click here to view
Table 8 Auditory P 300-evoked potential in patients and controls

Click here to view
Table 9 Auditory P 300-evoked potential in hepatitis B virus and hepatitis C virus patients

Click here to view


Auditory P300-evoked potential

It showed a significantly reduced P300 amplitude (P=0.011) and prolonged latencies (P=0.035) in patients with chronic hepatitis C and B compared with healthy controls with no significant difference between both groups of patients ([Table 8] and [Table 9]).

Correlates of cognitive impairment among cases with chronic hepatitis

Patients’ age, BMI, and viral load titer all showed no correlation with any of the cognitive tools used for the assessment of cognitive function among cases which are WMS, WCST, TMT, MOCA tests, and P300-evoked potential. However, BDI only showed a significant positive correlation with all mentioned cognitive battery tools; more depressive features were positively correlated with more cognitive impairment ([Table 10],[Table 11],[Table 12],[Table 13]).
Table 10 Correlation between patient age, BMI, Beck Depression Inventory, and virus load titer on one hand and Trail Making Test and Montreal Cognitive Assessment Test on the other hand

Click here to view
Table 11 Correlation between patient age, BMI, Beck Depression Inventory, and virus load titer on one hand and Wechsler Memory Scale on the other hand

Click here to view
Table 12 Correlation between patient age, BMI, Beck Depression Inventory, and virus load titer on one hand and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test on the other hand

Click here to view
Table 13 Correlation between patient age, BMI, Beck Depression Inventory, and virus load titer on one hand and auditory P300-evoked potential on the other hand

Click here to view



  Discussion Top


Forton et al. (2003) and Kramer et al. (2005) had reported that subclinical cognitive dysfunction proved by neuropsychological tests had been encountered in the vast majority of chronic hepatitis C-infected patients without evident sequelae on hepatic functions.

However, cognitive dysfunction in patients with chronic hepatitis precedes the process of cirrhosis and was not related to the degree of liver affection, the viral load titer, or the genotype (Forton et al., 2001, 2002).

Despite that the larger amount of studies in that theme reported the presence of cognitive impairment among the patients with chronic hepatitis; Cordoba and Soogoor reports denied any correlation of HCV and cognition. However, Córdoba et al. (2003) collected his sample from blood donation patients, while Soogoor et al. (2006) had a very young aged sample which means the two cases are in the early phase of illness.

Although the treatment with interferon, the concomitant substance abuse, the longer duration of illness (Foster, 2009) or higher hepatitis C viral load or HIV coinfection (Sun et al., 2013) were assumed to be responsible for chronic hepatitis-related cognitive impairment, the direct involvement of the brain was considered as well (Forton et al., 2001).

Recently, the hypothesis of HCV neuroinflammation without evident central nervous system (CNS) infection had received much emphasis and is considered as a probable explanation for what was termed MHE. Detection of sequences of the virus in CSF of a significant number of patients had been reported in some reports (Laskus et al., 2002). These virus strains were identical to that visualized in the peripheral mononuclear blood cells, which have the ability to cross the blood–brain barrier, hence, the possibility of brain infection by such strains through an immune-mediated mechanism starting with HCV-infected peripheral mononuclear blood cells, generation of microglial cells, and macrophages in the CNS and the production of excess brain levels of cytokines such as interleukin 8 and tumor necrosis factor α that represents the end of a cascade where cognitive impairment may be an outcome (Senzolo et al., 2011; Abrantes et al., 2013; Liu et al., 2014). Reduced production of brain-derived neurotrophic factor by the effect of HCV was assumed in some studies (Modabbernia et al., 2011).

More interesting was the recent fact that the microvascular endothelial cells of the brain expressed HCV receptors. This suggests the hypothesis that a low rate of replication of distinct strains of HCV occurs in the brain which may explain the disorder of MHE (Fletcher et al., 2012).

Furthermore, an additional MRI spectroscopy evidence of neuroinflammation was suggested (Chang et al., 2013; Zahr et al., 2014). Many suggestions were made for such findings: abnormal calcium homeostasis (Schwer et al., 2004), disturbance of monoamine neurotransmission (Forton et al., 2006), abnormal gene expression (Senzolo et al., 2011), cytokine immune response (Abrantes et al., 2013), or concomitant CNS infection (Abrantes et al., 2013).

Structural and functional neuroimaging-based evidence of structural and functional brain (frontal, basal ganglionic, and white matter) changes had been suggested in patients with chronic hepatitis C infection without liver decompensation (Posada et al., 2009).

The study of minimal cognitive impairment and its attribution to chronic hepatitis free of any other influencing factors may represent a matter of difficulty. This is because the extrahepatic manifestations of chronic hepatitis particularly the neuropsychiatric ones such as depression and fatigue are common among those patients. In some reports, depression was estimated to affect about one-third of chronic hepatitis C patients compared with the general population (Adinolfi et al., 2017). Although some authors went to report that depressive features were more common in chronic hepatitis C than in chronic hepatitis B (Qureshi et al., 2012; Mohamed et al., 2013), this study revealed no significant difference in the severity of depression between both groups. This concomitant depression might be intimately associated with at least minimal cognitive deficits (Lam et al., 2014) which might be attributed to depression rather than chronic hepatitis itself.

Depression occurs in all stages of illness even in the early stages and may be a reaction to the psychosocial stress of the illness (Yeoh et al., 2018). Incidence of depression and anxiety was reported to be higher in patients with chronic HCV and HBV infection in a large amount of old and recent studies either in de novo (Kasraian et al., 2016) or in interferon-treated patients (Huckans et al., 2015). In some researches, chronic hepatitis-related depression was restricted and positively correlated to the severity of cirrhosis (Zhu et al., 2016).

Psychosocial impact of chronic hepatitis infection including depression has many sources of origin among such patients; patients with chronic hepatitis have persistent fear about their fate of illness, its progress and its complications, feel stigmatized, and feel guilty about the possibility to infect their spouses or their kids (Tu and Shao, 2009). Moreover, the suspected completely curable nature of chronic hepatitis B makes the patients feel depressed and desperate (Fan et al., 2004).

In the last few years, the progress of antiviral treatments for hepatitis had achieved a better spread and success in lowering the prevalence of the disease all over the world. Interferon therapy had reached a large sector of diseased populations and was provided free to a large the disease-affected area all over the world. This makes the study on de novo cases a matter of difficulty and this makes testing of cognitive impairment either related to the disease or to the treatment a matter of great challenge. This is because interferon therapy itself was associated in many studies with cognitive impairment (Lieb et al., 2006; Tanaka and Sasaki, 2017) rather than other psychiatric side effects including depression, fatigue, anxiety, and pain (Huckans et al., 2015). Because of this, we targeted de novo cases for neuropsychological assessment to avoid such causative overlap.

Some researchers went to attribute such subtle impairment of cognitive function to the treatment with interferon alpha where the pretreatment cognitive and psychiatric assessment were unremarkable (Lieb et al., 2006), while others reported that the effect of interferon was restricted to the development of psychiatric symptoms such as depression, fatigue, and pain rather than the development of cognitive impairment (Amodioa et al., 2005; Huckans et al., 2015).

Most of the literature about the predilection of chronic hepatitis to impair cognition in asymptomatic patients had addressed the functional rather than the structural assessment. Recently, the combined approach had been addressed in the current rather than the backdated research. Novel functional MRI techniques had been used for achieving that purpose. Diffusion tensor imaging is an example of such novel techniques which has a specific value in detecting the microstructural changes in the brain through the measurement of metabolic and diffusion changes in different brain areas. Recently, Ajay Kumar had used a combined psychometric and anatomical approach in detecting microstructural changes in patients with chronic hepatitis using the diffusion tensor imaging MRI technique and, to our knowledge, it was the first study to address such combined approach. They reported decreased values of fractional anisotropy and myoinositol on inferior longitudinal bundles, and superior thalamic radiations in comparison to controls and this had a positive correlation with the lower performance on the cognitive psychometric tests (Kumar et al., 2017).

Due to its wider prevalence, HCV infection had received much more growing interest for research. However, Mohamed et al. (2013) compared the pattern of cognitive dysfunction related to HCV to that related to HBV infection which was shown to be similar in both groups.

Being considered as an index of severity, viral load titer was studied as a possible correlate of chronic hepatitis-related cognitive dysfunction. The viral load was considered as a correlate to chronic hepatitis-related cognitive impairment in some reports (Sun et al., 2013), while in this study and similarly in other previous reports (McAndrews et al., 2005; Mohamed et al., 2013), it did not reveal to be a relevant correlate. This controversy may be added to the amount of support of the hypothesis that the direct involvement of the brain by the virus may contribute to the pathogenesis of chronic hepatitis-related cognitive impairment which is not viral load dependent (Kramer et al., 2002, Perry et al., 2008).

Furthermore, the relationship between obesity and depression (Stunkard et al., 2003) and the relationship between chronic hepatitis and depression are common findings in the literature, so, the study of obesity as a separate correlate was logic and relevant. Although BMI showed a negative correlation with the cognitive functions of healthy people in some studies (Cournot et al., 2006), BMI showed no relationship with the cognitive impairment detected in patients with chronic hepatitis in this study. Hashemi et al. (2015), who reported that obesity was not a correlate of cognitive impairment in patients with chronic hepatitis, had explained that by the fact that the body weight was paradoxically correlated to the progress of illness, obese patients are mostly in the early phases of the disease where cognitive functions are still preserved.

Limitations of the study

The small size of the sample, the absence of liver biopsy as an important tool of evaluation of liver state, and the absence of advanced neuroimaging as a brain structure assessment tool are a group of study limitations that should be considered in the future work about that issue.


  Conclusion Top


Neuropsychological- and neurophysiological-based cognitive assessment in patients with chronic hepatitis C and B had been a target of many previous studies. A strong evidence of cognitive impairment among those patients was considered. This study was added to the body of support of such evidence. Further studies should be encouraged in such field for better quality of life and treatment outcome of such group of high-incidence patients.[53]

Financial support and sponsorship

Nil.

Conflicts of interest

There are no conflicts of interest.



 
  References Top

1.
Abrantes J, Torres DS, de Mello CE (2013). Patients with hepatitis C infection and normal liver function: an evaluation of cognitive function. Postgrad Med J 89:433–439.  Back to cited text no. 1
    
2.
Adinolfi LE, Nevola R, Rinaldi L, Romano C, Giordano M (2017). Chronic hepatitis C virus infection and depression. Clin Liver Dis 21:517–534.  Back to cited text no. 2
    
3.
Amodioa P, De Tonia EN, Cavallettoa L, Mapellib D, Bernardinelloa E, Del Piccoloa F et al. (2005). Mood, cognition and EEG changes during interferon a (alpha-IFN) treatment for chronic hepatitis C. J Affect Disord 84:93–98.  Back to cited text no. 3
    
4.
Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK (1996). Beck depression inventory manual. 2nd ed. New York: Psychological Corporation.  Back to cited text no. 4
    
5.
Blach S, Zeuzem S, Manns M, Altraif I, Duberg AS, Muljono DH et al. (2017). Global prevalence and genotype distribution of hepatitis C virus infection in 2015: a modelling study. Lancet Gastroenterol Hepatol 2:161–176.  Back to cited text no. 5
    
6.
Butters H, Salmon DP, Cullum CM, Cairns P, Tröster AI, Jacobs D et al. (1988). Differentiation of amnesic and demented patients with the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. Clin Neuropsychol 2:133–148.  Back to cited text no. 6
    
7.
Chang L, Munsaka SM, Kraft-Terry S, Ernst T (2013). Magnetic resonance spectroscopy to assess neuroinflammation and neuropathic pain. J Neuroimmune Pharmacol 8:576–593.  Back to cited text no. 7
    
8.
Cournot M, Marquié JC, Ansiau D, Martinaud C, Fonds H, Ferrières J, Ruidavets JB (2006). Relation between body mass index and cognitive function in healthy middle-aged men and women. Neurology 67:1208–14.  Back to cited text no. 8
    
9.
Córdoba J, Flavià M, Jacas C, Sauleda S, Esteban JI, Vargas V et al. (2003). Quality of life and cognitive function in hepatitis C at different stages of liver disease. J Hepatol 39:231–238.  Back to cited text no. 9
    
10.
Fan ZP, Wang FS, Xu DP, Chu FL, Shi M, Zhou Y, Zhang LX (2004). Detection of HBcAg-specific cytotoxic lymphocytes and their association with clinical status in patients with hepatitis B. Zhonghua Yi Xue Za Zhi 84:2073–2076.  Back to cited text no. 10
    
11.
Fletcher NF, Wilson GK, Murray J, Hu K, Lewis A, Reynolds GM et al. (2012). Hepatitis C virus infects the endothelial cells of the blood-brain barrier. Gastroenterology 142:634–643.  Back to cited text no. 11
    
12.
Forton DM, Allsop JM, Main J, Foster GR, Thomas HC, Taylor-Robinson SD (2001). Evidence for a cerebral effect of the hepatitis C virus. Lancet 358:38–39.  Back to cited text no. 12
    
13.
Forton DM, Thomas HC, Murphy CA (2002). Hepatitis C and cognitive impairment in a cohort of patients with mild liver disease. Hepatology 35:433–439.  Back to cited text no. 13
    
14.
Forton DM, Taylor-Robinson SD, Thomas HC (2003). Cerebral dysfunction in chronic hepatitis C infection. J Viral Hepat 10:81–86.  Back to cited text no. 14
    
15.
Forton DM, Taylor-Robinson SD, Thomas HC (2006). Central nervous system changes in hepatitis C virus infection. Eur J Gastroenterol Hepatol 18:333–338.  Back to cited text no. 15
    
16.
Foster GR (2009). Quality of life considerations for patients with chronic hepatitis C. J Viral Hepat 16:605–11.  Back to cited text no. 16
    
17.
Hashemi F, Ashtiani AF, Mirminachi B, Sharafkhah M, Ekhlasi G, Jafari E, Poustchi H (2015). Impact of hepatitis C virus infection on cognitive function in patients with covert hepatic encephalopathy. Hepat Mon 15:e30507.  Back to cited text no. 17
    
18.
Heaton RK, Chelune GJ, Talley JL, Kay GG, Curtiss G (1993). Wisconsin card sorting test manual: Revised and expanded. Odessa, TX: Psychological Assessment Resources.  Back to cited text no. 18
    
19.
Huckans M, Fuller B, Wheaton V, Jaehnert S, Ellis C, Kolessar M et al. (2015). A longitudinal study evaluating the effects of interferon-alpha therapy on cognitive and psychiatric function in adults with chronic hepatitis C. J Psychosom Res 78:184–192.  Back to cited text no. 19
    
20.
Kasraian L, Negarestani N, Karimi MH, Dehbidi S (2016). A survey on the prevalence of depression in blood donors with hepatitis C in Shiraz. Hepat Mon 16:e31080.  Back to cited text no. 20
    
21.
Kramer L, Bauer E, Funk G, Hofer H, Jessner W, Steindl-Munda P et al. (2002). Subclinical impairment of brain function in chronic hepatitis C infection. J Hepatol 37:349–354.  Back to cited text no. 21
    
22.
Kramer L, Hofer H, Bauer E, Funk G, Formann E, Steindl-Munda P, Ferenci P (2005). Relative impact of fatigue and subclinical cognitive brain dysfunction on health-related quality of life in chronic hepatitis C infection. AIDS 19(Suppl 3):S85–S92.  Back to cited text no. 22
    
23.
Kumar A, Deep A, Gupta RK, Atam V, Mohindra S (2017). Brain microstructural correlates of cognitive dysfunction in clinically and biochemically normal hepatitis C virus infection. J Clin Exp Hepatol 7:198–204.  Back to cited text no. 23
    
24.
Lam RW, Kennedy SH, McIntyre RS, Khullar A (2014). Cognitive dysfunction in major depressive disorder: effects on psychosocial functioning and implications for treatment. Can J Psychiatry 59:649–654.  Back to cited text no. 24
    
25.
Laskus T, Radkowski M, Bednarska A, Wilkinson J, Adair D, Nowicki M et al. (2002). Detection and analysis of hepatitis C virus sequences in cerebrospinal fluid. J Virol 76:10064–10068.  Back to cited text no. 25
    
26.
Lezak MD (2004). Neuropsychological assessment. 4th ed. New York: Oxford University Press.  Back to cited text no. 26
    
27.
Lieb K, Engelbrecht MA, Gut O, Fiebich BL, Bauer J, Janssen G, Schaefer M (2006). Cognitive impairment in patients with chronic hepatitis treated with interferon alpha (IFNalpha): results from a prospective study. Eur Psychiatry 21:204–10.  Back to cited text no. 27
    
28.
Liu Z, Zhao F, He JJ (2014). Hepatitis C virus (HCV) interaction with astrocytes: nonproductive infection and induction of IL-18. J Neurovirol 20: 278–293.  Back to cited text no. 28
    
29.
McAndrews MP, Farcnik K, Carlen P, Damyanovich A, Mrkonjic M, Jones S, Heathcote EJ (2005). Prevalence and significance of neurocognitive dysfunction in hepatitis C in the absence of correlated risk factors. Hepatology 41:801–808.  Back to cited text no. 29
    
30.
Ministry of Health and Population [Egypt], El-Zanaty and Associates [Egypt] & and ICF International (2015). Egypt Health Issues Survey 2015. Cairo, Egypt and Rockville, MD: Ministry of Health and Population and ICF International.  Back to cited text no. 30
    
31.
Modabbernia A, Ashrafi M, Keyvani H, Taslimi S, Poorkaveh A, Merat S et al. (2011). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor predicts physical health in untreated patients with hepatitis C. Biol Psychiatry 70:e31–2.  Back to cited text no. 31
    
32.
Mohamed HI, Fath-Elbab HK, Rabie SM, Abel-Hamid WM (2013). Assessment of cognitive functions in patients with chronic liver diseases using neuropsychological test battery. J Med Med Sci 4:167–173.  Back to cited text no. 32
    
33.
Monaco S, Ferrari S, Gajofatto A, Zanusso G, Mariotto S (2012). HCV-related nervous system disorders. Clin Dev Immunol 2012:236148.  Back to cited text no. 33
    
34.
Nasreddine ZS, Collin I, Chertkow H, Phillips N, Bergman H, Whitehead V (2003). Sensitivity and specificity of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) for detection of mild cognitive deficits. Can J Neurol Sci 30(Suppl.2):30.  Back to cited text no. 34
    
35.
Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I et al. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA): a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 53:695–699.  Back to cited text no. 35
    
36.
Owiti JA, Greenhalgh T, Sweeney L, Foster GR, Bhui KS (2015). Illness perceptions and explanatory models of viral hepatitis B & C among immigrants and refugees: a narrative systematic review. BMC Public Health 15:151.  Back to cited text no. 36
    
37.
Perry W, Hilsabeck RC, Hassanein TI (2008). Cognitive dysfunction in chronic hepatitis C: a review. Dig Dis Sci 53(2):307–321.  Back to cited text no. 37
    
38.
Polich J, Kok A (1995). Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: an integrative review. Biol Psychol 41(2):103–146.  Back to cited text no. 38
    
39.
Posada C, Morgan EE, Moore DJ et al. (2009) Neurocognitive effects of the hepatitis C virus. Curr Hepatitis Rep 8:158–166.  Back to cited text no. 39
    
40.
Qureshi M, Khokhar N, Shafqat F (2012). Severity of depression in hepatitis B and hepatitis C patients. J Coll Phys Surg 22:632–634.  Back to cited text no. 40
    
41.
Reitan RM (1958). Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept Motor Skills 8:271–276.  Back to cited text no. 41
    
42.
Schwer B, Ren S, Pietschmann T, Kartenbeck J, Kaehlcke K, Bartenschlager R et al. (2004). Targeting of hepatitis C virus core protein to mitochondria through a novel C-terminal localization motif. J Virol 78(15):7958–7968.  Back to cited text no. 42
    
43.
Senzolo M, Schiff S, D’Aloisio CM, Crivellin C, Cholongitas E, Burra P, Montagnese S. (2011). Neuropsychological alterations in hepatitis C infection: the role of inflammation. World J Gastroenterol 17:3369–74.  Back to cited text no. 43
    
44.
Soogoor M, Lynn HS, Donfield SM, Gomperts E, Bell TS, Daar ES (2006). Hemophilia growth and development study. Hepatitis C virus infection and neurocognitive function. Neurology 24:1482–1485.  Back to cited text no. 44
    
45.
Stunkard AJ, Faith MS, Allison KC (2003). Depression and obesity. Biol Psychiatry 54:330–337.  Back to cited text no. 45
    
46.
Sun B, Abadjian L, Rempel H, Monto A, Pulliam L (2013). Differential cognitive impairment in HCV coinfected men with controlled HIV compared to HCV monoinfection. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr 62:190–196.  Back to cited text no. 46
    
47.
Tanaka H, Sasaki H (2017). Cognitive impairment with interferon treatment in patients with chronic hepatitis C. Biomed Res 38:371–374.  Back to cited text no. 47
    
48.
Tu B, Shao W (2009). Chronic hepatitis B with depression treatment efficacy ademetionine. Zhong Xi Yi Jie He Gan Bing Za Zhi 19:174–175.  Back to cited text no. 48
    
49.
Vilstrup H, Amodio P, Bajaj J, Cordoba J, Ferenci P, Mullen KD et al. (2014). Hepatic encephalopathy in chronic liver disease: 2014 Practice Guideline by the American Association for the Study of Liver Diseases and the European Association for the Study of the Liver. Hepatology 60:715–735.  Back to cited text no. 49
    
50.
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) is a widely used psychometric tool for the assessment of executive functioning which was first developed by Grant and Berg (1948).  Back to cited text no. 50
    
51.
Yeoh SW, Holmes ACN, Saling MM, Everall IP, Nicoll AJ (2018). Depression, fatigue and neurocognitive deficits in chronic hepatitis C. Hepatol Int 12:294–304.  Back to cited text no. 51
    
52.
Zahr NM, Mayer D, Rohlfing T, Sullivan EV, Pfefferbaum A (2014). Imaging neuroinflammation? A perspective from MR spectroscopy. Brain Pathol 24:654–664.  Back to cited text no. 52
    
53.
Zhu HP, Gu YR, Zhang GL, Su YJ, Wang KE, Zheng YB, Gao ZL (2016). Depression in patients with chronic hepatitis B and cirrhosis is closely associated with the severity of liver cirrhosis. Exp Ther Med 12:405–409.  Back to cited text no. 53
    



 
 
    Tables

  [Table 1], [Table 2], [Table 3], [Table 4], [Table 5], [Table 6], [Table 7], [Table 8], [Table 9], [Table 10], [Table 11], [Table 12], [Table 13]



 

Top
 
 
  Search
 
Similar in PUBMED
   Search Pubmed for
   Search in Google Scholar for
 Related articles
Access Statistics
Email Alert *
Add to My List *
* Registration required (free)

 
  In this article
Abstract
Introduction
Aim of the work
Patients and methods
Results
Discussion
Conclusion
References
Article Tables

 Article Access Statistics
    Viewed2071    
    Printed189    
    Emailed0    
    PDF Downloaded161    
    Comments [Add]    

Recommend this journal


[TAG2]
[TAG3]
[TAG4]